The analysis and an independent commentary on the findings appear in the journal Psychological Science in the Public Interest.
«The idea behind ‘brain training’ is that if you practice a task that taps a core component of cognitive ability, like memory, the training will improve your ability to perform other tasks that also rely on memory, not just in the lab, but also in the world. That premise is known as ‘
«If you practice remembering playing cards, you’ll get really good at remembering playing cards," Simons said. «But does that help you remember which medications to take, and when? Does it help you remember your friends’ names? Historically, there is not much evidence that practicing one task improves different tasks in other contexts, even if they seem to rely on the same ability.»
The researchers closely examined 132 journal articles cited by a large group of
The review found numerous problems with the way many of the cited studies were designed and how the evidence was reported and interpreted. The problems included small sample sizes and studies in which researchers reported only a handful of significant results from the many measures collected.
«Sometimes the effects of a single
Some studies conducted with special groups (such as people diagnosed with schizophrenia, children with language delays, or older adults with dementia) were used as support for broad claims about the benefits of brain training for the general population.
One of the most glaring problems in the cited research was the use of inadequate control groups as a baseline for measuring improvements. Ideally, participants in a control group do not engage in the intervention but are otherwise matched closely with those who do, the researchers said. Not only should the control group’s demographics (age, sex, race, income and education) match those of the intervention group as closely as possible,
«A control group should experience everything the treatment group does, except for the critical ingredient of the treatment," he said. «They should be equally engaged and should have similar expectations for improvement, so that if the treatment group improves more than the control group, the difference must be due to the treatment itself.»
Some of the studies had no control group. Some had a passive control group, whose members took the same pre- and
Most of the cited research tested for improvements on simplified, abstract laboratory tasks rather than on measures of
«There are relatively few studies in this literature that objectively measure improvements on the sorts of
«Based on our comprehensive review of the evidence cited by
The research team included Walter Boot and Neil Charness, of Florida State University; Susan Gathercole, of the Medical Research Council, Cambridge, U.K.; Christopher Chabris, of Union College and Geisinger Health System; and David Hambrick, of Michigan State University. Simons and